Hossein Nassaji - The Relationship between Depth of Vocabulary Knowledge and L2 Learners' Lexical Inferencing Strategy Use and Success - Canadian Modern Language Review / La Revue Canadienne des Langues Vivantes 61:1 Canadian Modern Language Review / La Revue Canadienne des Langues Vivantes 61.1 (2004) 107-134

The Relationship between Depth of Vocabulary Knowledge and L2 Learners' Lexical Inferencing Strategy Use and Success

Abstract
This study examines the relationship between ESL learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge, their lexical inferencing strategy use, and their success in deriving word meaning from context. Participants read a passage containing 10 unknown words and attempted to derive the meanings of the unknown words from context. Introspective think-aloud protocols were used to discover the degree and types of inferencing strategies learners used. The Word-Associate Test (WAT) (Read, 1993) was used to measure the learner's depth of vocabulary knowledge. Results indicate a significant relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and the degree and type of strategy use and success. They reveal that (a) those who had stronger depth of vocabulary knowledge used certain strategies more frequently than those who had weaker depth of vocabulary knowledge; (b) the stronger students made more effective use of certain types of lexical inferencing strategies than their weaker counterparts; and (c) depth of vocabulary knowledge made a significant contribution to inferential success over and above the contribution made by the learner's degree of strategy use. These findings provide empirical support for the centrality of depth of vocabulary knowledge in lexical inferencing and the hypothesis that lexical inferencing is a meaning construction process that is significantly influenced by the richness of the learner's pre-existing semantic system.

Résumé
Cette étude se penche sur le rapport entre le niveau de connaissance du vocabulaire d'apprenants en anglais langue seconde, l'utilisation de stratégies d'inférences lexicales et le succès des apprenants à déchiffrer la signification de mots à partir du contexte. Les participants ont lu un extrait renfermant 10 mots inconnus et ont cherché à en découvrir le sens en se servant du contexte. On a eu recours aux protocoles d'introspection verbaux afin de déterminer la quantité et le genre de stratégies d'inférence utilisées par les apprenants. Le test d'association de mots WAT (Read, 1993) a été utilisé pour mesurer le niveau de profondeur du vocabulaire. Les résultats [End Page 107] ont révélé une corrélation significative entre ce dernier ainsi que la quantité et la variété de stratégies utilisées avec succès. On a trouvé, en particulier, que (a) les individus qui étaient plus compétents en matière de vocabulaire (profondeur) se servaient plus fréquemment de certaines stratégies que les moins compétents; (b) les étudiants plus forts utilisaient plus efficacement certains types de stratégies d'inférences lexicales que les plus faibles; (c) le niveau de connaissance du vocabulaire (profondeur) contribuait largement au succès des inférences, et ce, de façon plus significative que le degré d'utilisation des stratégies par l'apprenant. Ces conclusions confirment le rôle primordial joué par le vocabulaire (profondeur) dans la mise en oeuvre d'inférences lexicales nettement influencées par la richesse du système sémantique préexistant de l'apprenant.

Introduction

One of the central cognitive processes in reading comprehension is inferencing (R.C. Anderson & Pearson, 1984; Graesser & Bower, 1990; Kintsch, 1988, 1998; Monzo & Calvo, 2002; Nassaji, 2002, 2003a, 2003b; Whitney, 1987). Inferencing has been defined as the connections that people establish when they try to interpret texts (G. Brown & Yule, 1983). Inferencing occurs at all levels of the reading comprehension process, ranging from integrating the text with background knowledge (Kintsch, 1988), to connecting the different parts of the text together (Garrod & Sanford, 1990; Kintsch, 1988, 1998), to linking known to unknown elements in the text in order to arrive at a coherent structure of the information in the text (Garrod & Sanford, 1990; Graesser & Bower, 1990; Graesser & Zwaan, 1995). Such processes are assumed to involve prediction and interpretation of the text for meaning; hence, they are considered important processes by theories in cognitive psychology that conceptualize reading as an active meaning-construction process and a creation of a mental representation of the text (Kintsch, 1988, 1998).

The present study focused on lexical inferencing, that is, making 'informed guesses' about the meaning of unknown words based on the available linguistic and non-linguistic cues in the text (Haastrup, 1991, p. 40). Lexical inferencing has been found to be widely used by second language (L2) learners when dealing with unknown words in their reading (de Bot, Paribakht, & Wesche, 1997; Frantzen, 2003; Fraser, 1999; Huckin & Bloch, 1993; Morrison, 1996; Paribakht & Wesche, 1999; Parry, 1993). Lexical inferencing has also been found to be closely associated with incidental vocabulary learning, that is, learning vocabulary through reading natural texts (Huckin & Coady, 1999; Nagy, 1997). [End Page 108] Thus, Wesche and Paribakht (1999) argue that 'much - if not most - lexical development in both L1 and L2 appears to occur as learners attempt to comprehend new words they hear or read in context' (p. 176).

Although researchers have attributed an important role to lexical inferencing, the nature of this process has not been well understood in second language acquisition (SLA) (Paribakht & Wesche, 1999). To this end, and given the important role currently attributed to L2 learners' lexical knowledge in L2 reading comprehension (Laufer, 1996, 1997; Nassaji, 2002, 2003a; Nation, 2001; Qian, 2002; Read, 1993, 1997, 2000), the present study examined the particular role learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge plays in lexical inferencing. The question addressed was, How does L2 learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge relate to the degree and the type of the lexical inferencing strategies they use, and how does this relationship affect the learners' success in deriving word meaning from context?

Factors affecting learners' success in lexical inferencing

Many factors have been shown to affect success in lexical inferencing, including the nature of the word and the text that contains the word (Paribakht & Wesche, 1999; Parry, 1993); the degree of textual information available in the surrounding context (Dubin & Olshtain, 1993), the learner's ability to make use of extra-textual cues (de Bot et al., 1997; Haastrup, 1991); the importance of the word to comprehension of the text (C.M. Brown, 1993); the degree of cognitive and mental effort involved in the task (Fraser, 1999; Joe, 1995); and the learner's attention to the details in the text as well as his or her preconceptions about the possible meaning of the word (Frantzen, 2003). In a discussion of the factors involved in lexical inferencing, Nagy (1997) considers the role of learners' pre-existing knowledge bases and how these knowledge bases influence learners' strategy use and success. Nagy groups learners' knowledge bases into three main categories: linguistic knowledge, world knowledge, and strategic knowledge. The linguistic knowledge category covers all knowledge that learners possess about the linguistic context in which the word has occurred, including their syntactic knowledge, lexical knowledge, and knowledge of word schema (i.e., knowledge of the possible meanings of the word). World knowledge is the learner's understanding and use of the relevant domains of knowledge. Strategic knowledge is knowledge of the actual strategies learners employ during the act of inferencing and attempting to deduce the meaning of the unknown word from context. Nagy suggests that strategic knowledge may not be necessary for acquiring word meaning from context, but that [End Page 109] sometimes, when the learner is aware of the existence of new words in the text, he or she may make deliberate attempts to derive the meanings of these words from context. Nagy then reviews several studies that have highlighted the importance of such strategic attempts in deriving word meaning from context (Buikema & Graves, 1993; Huckin & Jin, 1987; Jenkins, Matlock, & Slocum, 1989).

Similarly, in a discussion of what is involved in successful strategy use, Pressley, Borkowski, and Schneider (1987) propose a framework that distinguishes between a cognitive strategic component, which includes a repertoire of general as well as domain-specific strategies learners have, and a knowledge-base component, including various knowledge bases constructed from learners' various experiences with the world. These knowledge bases range from well-established and integrated pieces of information about particular phenomena or situations, to knowledge about specific strategies and skills, to knowledge about when and how to apply these in a particular situation. Knowledge the strategy user needs to evaluate the causes of his or her failure and the relationship between his efforts and achievement also fall into this category.

In the context of L2 lexical inferencing, and based on an exploratory study with intermediate ESL learners, Huckin and Bloch (1993) propose a lexical inferencing model that incorporates similar components. These components include a knowledge module component (e.g., a vocabulary knowledge module, a text schema module, a syntax and morphology module, and a text representation module) and a metalinguistic strategic component. The metalinguistic strategic component includes a sequence of cognitive and decision-making strategies that the learner uses when trying to generate and test word meanings and hypotheses. According to Huckin and Bloch, these strategies play an important role in lexical inferencing in that they help the learner decide when and how to proceed and seek help from context and various sources of knowledge available.

The above frameworks underscore the multidimensionality of strategy use. In particular, they highlight the fact that multiple knowledge sources and strategies (i.e., the various cognitive and metacognitive activities learners use when identifying and constructing word meaning from context) are involved in inferencing word meanings from context. A number of recent studies have documented the range of knowledge sources and strategies learners employ during lexical inferencing (Chern, 1993; de Bot et al, 1997; Haynes, 1993, Huckin & Bloch, 1993; Morrison, 1996; Paribakht & Wesche, 1999). For example, in a study with university students, de Bot et al. (1997) found that when attempting to [End Page 110] infer word meaning from context, L2 readers used knowledge sources ranging from knowledge of grammar, morphology, phonology, and knowledge of the world, to knowledge of punctuation, word association, and cognates. Analyzing the lexical inferencing strategies of Danish learners of English, Haastrup (1991) found that learners used different strategies ranging from those related to the internal structure of the word (such as analysis of the phonological and orthographic structure of the word) to those involving the use of top-down contextual and sentence-level clues.

Among the knowledge sources, one type of knowledge source that has been found to be strongly related to the learner's ability to read and understand texts is vocabulary knowledge. A number of studies in both L1 and L2 have demonstrated that vocabulary knowledge is one the best predictors of reading ability and the ability to acquire new information from texts (J.C. Anderson, 2000; Nation, 2001; Qian, 2002; Read, 2000). Several studies have also demonstrated a relationship between learners' vocabulary knowledge and their subsequent learning of vocabulary through reading (Haynes & Baker, 1993; Parry 1997; Pulido, 2003; Sternberg & Powell, 1983; Verspoor & Lowie, 2003). In a study with L1 learners, for example, Sternberg and Powell (1983) found a positive relationship between learners' performance on a vocabulary test and their ability to define word meanings. In a recent study with Dutch learners of English, Verspoor and Lowie (2003) found a similar relationship. The researchers found that learners' ability to infer and retain the meanings of polysemous words from context was related significantly to their knowledge of the core sense of the word. In another study with L2 learners, Pulido (2003) found that knowledge of sight vocabulary was significantly correlated with measures of incidental vocabulary gains from reading.

Depth versus breadth of vocabulary knowledge

In research on vocabulary learning, a distinction has often been made between two dimensions of vocabulary knowledge: depth of knowledge and size, or breadth, of knowledge (Haastrup & Henriksen, 2000; Meara, 1996; Read, 2000). Breadth of vocabulary knowledge has been taken to refer to the quantity or number of words learners know at a particular level of language proficiency (Nation, 2001). Researchers have used various types of assessment tools with different formats to measure this dimension of vocabulary knowledge, including tests that require the learner to identify a synonym for a word in a multiple-choice test, match words with definitions, translate a word into L1, or use checklists (see [End Page 111] Wesche & Paribakht, 1996, for a discussion of these various assessment types). One measure that has been widely used to assess size of vocabulary knowledge in the literature, for example, is Nation's Vocabulary Level test (1990, 1993), which has a word-meaning matching format and is composed of words representing different word frequency levels, ranging from high-frequency (2,000 word level) to low-frequency words (10,000 word level).

Depth of vocabulary knowledge, on the other hand, has been used to refer to the quality of lexical knowledge, or how well the learner knows a word (Meara, 1996; Read, 1993, 2000). Researchers have noted the complexity and multidimensionality of word knowledge and have suggested that knowing a word well should mean more than knowing its individual meanings in particular contexts. Various kinds of knowledge are associated with a word that a learner must know, ranging from knowledge related to its pronunciation, spelling, register, stylistic, and morphological features (Haastrup & Henriksen, 2000; Meara, 1996; Nation, 1990; Richards, 1976) to knowledge of the word's syntactic and semantic relationships with other words in the language, including collocational meanings and knowledge of antonymy, synonymy, and hyponymy (Chapelle, 1994; Henriksen, 1999; Read, 2000).

Studies investigating the role of vocabulary knowledge in reading have found that while measures of size of vocabulary knowledge are strongly related to the reader's understanding of texts (Laufer, 1997; Qian, 1998, 1999), measures examining aspects of depth of vocabulary knowledge make a stronger contribution to reading performance than those that simply measure a single definition of a word. Qian (1999), for example, found that depth of vocabulary knowledge, conceptualized as receptive knowledge of word meanings and collocations, was not only a better predictor of L2 reading comprehension but also made a unique contribution to L2 reading comprehension, over and above the contribution made by size of vocabulary knowledge.

In the present study, expanding on this line of research, I attempted to discover the role of L2 learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge in L2 lexical inferencing. In particular, I examined how L2 learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge relates to the degree and type of lexical inferencing strategy use and how this relationship mediates learners' success in deriving word meaning from context. I also investigated the degree to which depth of vocabulary knowledge and degree of lexical inferencing strategy use contribute, collectively and independently, to learners' inferential success. If it could be shown that lexical inferencing strategies make any contribution to lexical inferencing success this could have important implications for models of L2 vocabulary instruction that [End Page 112] advocate the use of lexical inferencing strategy in L2 vocabulary acquisition.

Method

The data examined in the present study come from a larger project on lexical inferencing strategies and knowledge sources. The study was carried out in two phases. In Phase 1, data were gathered and analyzed for the relationship between learners' lexical inferencing strategies and knowledge sources and their lexical inferencing success (Nassaji, 2003b). Phase 2 gathered data about learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge and analyzed them to discover its relationship with lexical inferencing strategy use and success.

Participants

Participants were 21 adult intermediate ESL learners from different language backgrounds, including Chinese, Spanish, Persian, Portuguese, and Arabic. All had recently arrived in Canada and were taking ESL courses to improve their English. They had met Level 4 of the Canadian Language Benchmark in listening and reading (Citizenship and Immigration Canada, 1996).

Depth of vocabulary knowledge test

As discussed earlier, depth of vocabulary knowledge is complex, and it is therefore very difficult to assess all the different components that constitute the full range of meanings and meaning relationships of a word. For the same reason, most vocabulary tests in the literature deal with breadth of vocabulary knowledge. In recent years, however, there have been some attempts to develop measures to assess aspects of depth of vocabulary knowledge (Greidanus & Nienhuis, 2001; Paribakht & Wesche, 1993; Read, 1993; Wesche & Paribakht, 1996). For example, in a university ESL context, Paribakht and Wesche (1993, 1996) developed a test called the Vocabulary Knowledge Scale (VKS), which was designed to measure the different levels of lexical knowledge of specific target words learners were learning in a comprehension-based ESL program. The test involves a self-report format in which learners are presented with individual words and asked to indicate their degree of knowledge on a scale of 1 to 5, ranging from no familiarity with the target word to the ability to use it accurately in a sentence. [End Page 113]

The VKS test has an easy format that can be used as a practical tool to measure the initial stages of word knowledge for certain words. However, as Wesche and Paribakht (1996) point out, the test was not designed to 'tap sophisticated knowledge of given words or describe the mental lexical networks to which the word belongs' (p. 29). Thus, the measure does not assess the various meaning relationships a word has with other words, though these have been suggested to be an important aspect of depth of vocabulary knowledge (Read, 2000). One measure that attempts to capture these lexical meaning relationships, and which was used in the present study, is the Word-Associate Test (WAT), developed by Read (1993). The WAT purports to measure the learner's depth of vocabulary knowledge through word associations, that is, the various semantic and collocational relationships that a word has with other words in the language. The test has been found to be closely correlated with L2 reading comprehension ability and has also been shown to have a high degree of internal reliability (Qian, 1999, 2002). The test used in the present study was version 3.1 (Form B)1 of the WAT. The test is composed of 50 target words, each followed by a list of eight words, four of which are semantically related to the target word while the other four are not. The four related words have been selected to represent three main types of semantic relationship with the target word: paradigmatic relationships (i.e., the word and its associate have similar meanings, such as 'enable' and 'allow'); syntagmatic relationships (i.e., the two words are collocates and co-occur in similar contexts, such as 'income' and 'tax'); and analytic relationships (i.e., the associate represents the meaning of part of the word, such as 'team' and 'together'). The reliability of the test (KR-20), as reported by Read (1993), is 0.92. (Further information about the test and how it was developed can be found in Read, 1993, 1997, 2000.)

The test was administered during a class period. Before learners took the test, they were notified of the general purpose of the study and were informed that their performance on the test would not affect their course outcome. They were instructed to read each of the target words and then circle the four words closely related to the target word. The time allocated to the test was 30 minutes. The split-half reliability of the test in the current study was 0.89.

Lexical inferencing strategies

To gather data about learners' lexical inferencing strategies, the students were presented with a reading passage and asked to read the text for comprehension and to try to infer the meanings of the unknown words. [End Page 114] Research suggests that successful inferencing depends heavily on the ability to comprehend the text as a whole and most of the words in it (Hirsh & Nation, 1992; Laufer, 1988; Liu & Nation, 1985). To meet these requirements, several passages, including those used in previous research, were examined. The reading passage selected for use in this study was the one developed by Haastrup (1991) in a study on lexical inferencing with Danish learners of English. The passage contained 374 words, with 10 target words highlighted (see Appendix). The passage had been designed to elicit the use of a variety of inferencing strategies and processes, ranging from those involved in the use of non-linguistic global comprehension processes to those involved in the use and integration of word-level cues such as prefixes and affixes (Haastrup, 1991). Before being used in the present study, the passage was pilot-tested with a group of ESL students assumed to have similar language proficiency to the participants in the main study. The pilot study revealed that the students had a good overall comprehension of the text (mean of comprehension: 7.6/10). It also showed that the percentage of unknown words in the passage ranged from 4.27% to 2.67%, derived by dividing the total number of the words reported as unknown by the total number of words in the passage and multiplying the results by 100.

An introspective think-aloud technique was used to discover the lexical inferencing strategies learners used; in this procedure, learners are asked to verbalize the content of their thoughts while attempting to infer the meaning of an unknown word from context. Data were collected in individual sessions lasting about 45-60 minutes. In each session, the students were first trained as to how to think aloud: they were given a set of pictures and asked to report what they thought was happening in the pictures. They were then presented with an English text and were asked to practise verbalizing their thinking while trying to infer the meanings of the unfamiliar words they encountered. After this practice session, the students were presented with the text intended for the study and were asked to read the text for comprehension and try to verbalize their thoughts when attempting to infer the meaning of the new words in the text.

Analysis and results

Identification of lexical inferencing strategies

In order to identify the lexical inferencing strategies used by these learners, all the introspective think-aloud protocols were initially transcribed and then carefully examined for any observable inferencing strategies. Lexical inferencing strategies were defined as any cognitive [End Page 115] or metacognitive activity that the learner turned to for help while trying to derive the meaning of the unknown word from context. Strategies were identified using an inductive procedure involving reading and rereading the protocols. The strategies identified derive mainly from the data and reflect the thinking of the learners participating in the study. Initially, three main categories of strategy types were identified. Following Pressley and Afferbach (1995), these were characterized as identifying, evaluating, and monitoring strategies. Identifying strategies were defined as those that the learners used to identify the meaning of the new word in the text. Learners were found to use different procedures to do so (e.g., repeating the word, repeating the section that contained the word, conducting word analysis, or word-form analogy); each was coded separately. A strategy was coded as word repeating when the learner repeated the word alone; as section repeating when the learner repeated a bigger section, including the clause or the sentence, in which the word had occurred; and as word-form analogy when the learner tried to identify the word meaning based on the sound or form similarity of the word to other words. It was coded as word analysis when the learner attempted to analyze the word into its different components to figure out its meaning.

Evaluating strategies were those that learners used to evaluate and check the accuracy of their initial inferences. Two types of sub-strategies were identified and coded in this category: self-inquiry, coded when learners questioned their initial inferences, and verifying, coded when learners reread a section of the text to revise or re-evaluate their initial inferencing hypotheses or the accuracy of their choices. A strategy was coded as monitoring when the learner showed an awareness of the nature of the problem by making an explicit judgement about the ease or difficulty of the word based on the available cues in the text. While both identifying and evaluating are cognitive strategies, monitoring is a metacognitive strategy.

The reliability of the coding was established by calculating an inter-coder agreement on a sample of 20% of the data, selected from every fifth participant and coded by the researcher and a second coder, a colleague of the researcher. The inter-coder agreement for that 20% of the data was 89%. Table 1 presents a taxonomy of these strategies, along with their definitions and examples from the transcripts.

Relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and successful inferencing

In order to determine the relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and lexical inferencing success, a variety of statistical procedures [End Page 116]

Types of strategies used2
Click for larger view
Table 1
Types of strategies used2

were employed. As a first step in the analysis, the degree of success in lexical inferencing was determined using a three-point scale (0 to 2) representing unsuccessful, partially successful, and successful attempts. Successful inferences were defined as those that were semantically, syntactically, and contextually appropriate. Responses that were semantically appropriate but syntactically deviant, or vice versa, were classified as partially successful. In order not to underestimate the learner's success, if the meaning or the definition provided made sense in the context but, when judged out of context, was not the meaning of the word, the attempt was still classified as partially successful. In cases where the response did not meet any of the above conditions, it was considered unsuccessful. All responses were rated and judged by two independent raters (the researcher and an ESL teacher who is a native English speaker), and an inter-rater reliability of 94% was established. The total number of responses to all target lexical items was 210 (10 target words (21 participants). However, during the reading and the [End Page 117] retrospective interviews, some participants reported that they knew some of the target words. This reduced the number of responses to 199.

Relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and inferential success
Click for larger view
Table 2
Relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and inferential success

As a second step in the analysis, the participants were divided into two groups: lexically skilled and lexically less skilled students. Grouping was done based on the participants' percentile rank for the scores they obtained on the depth of vocabulary knowledge test (WAT). A learner's percentile rank shows how well he or she performs on a test in comparison to others. Those learners whose scores fell at or below the 50th percentile were classified as lexically less skilled (LLS) readers, while those whose scores fell above the 50th percentile were classified as lexically skilled (LS) readers.There were 10 students in the LS group and 11 students in the LLS group.In the next step, the frequencies and percentages of successful, partially successful, and unsuccessful inferences were tallied. These frequencies were calculated for each of the students in each of the two groups of LS and LLS readers.

The results of these analyses are presented in Table 2. As the table shows, of a total of 51 successful inferences, 35 (68.6%) were made by LS readers and only 16 (31.4%) by LLS readers. Of the total number of unsuccessful inferences (111), a great proportion (60.4%) was made by LLS readers, and only 39.6% by LS readers. As for the partially successful inferences, the LLS group seems to have produced comparable numbers of such inferences, although the LS readers tended to produce slightly more. A two-way chi-square test conducted on the proportions of unsuccessful, partially successful, and successful inferences for the two groups of LS and LLS readers showed that the proportions of successful, partially successful, and unsuccessful inferences were significantly different across the two groups, indicating that the degree to which readers were able to infer word meaning successfully was related to their group membership, that is, whether they were lexically skilled or less skilled readers (x2 = 11.85, df = 2, p < 0.01). [End Page 118]

Relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and type of strategy use and success

The next stage of analysis examined the relationships between depth of vocabulary knowledge and strategy types and success. To this end, the proportion and the mean of inferencing success for each strategy across the two groups of LS and LLS readers were calculated. Means of success for each strategy were obtained by dividing the sum of each learner's scores (on the scale of 0 to 2) by the frequency of the strategy used. The resulting data were then compared across the two groups. T-tests and chi-square statistics were used to determine whether there were any significant differences between the two groups. Table 3 displays the results of these analyses.

Types of strategy use and success in lexically skilled versus lexically less skilled readers
Click for larger view
Table 3
Types of strategy use and success in lexically skilled versus lexically less skilled readers

As the table shows, LS and LLS students differed significantly in their overall rate of success in inferring word meaning from context. The overall mean of success for the LS group is significantly higher than [End Page 119] that for the LLS group (1.11 vs. 0.58, p < 0.000). Lexically skilled and less skilled readers also differed in terms of the use and efficacy of different types of strategies. LLS readers made more frequent use of word repeating than did LS readers (51.9% vs. 48.1%). The mean of success of this strategy, however, was significantly higher for the LS than for the LLS readers (0.85 vs. 0.47). On the other hand, the LS readers made more frequent use of section repeating than did the LLS readers (59.3% vs. 40.7%), and the mean of success for this strategy was also significantly higher for the former than for the latter group (1.20 vs. 0.82). In contrast, LLS readers made more frequent use of word analysis and word-form analogy than did the LS readers (word analysis: 73.1% vs. 26.9%; word-form analogy: 75.0% vs. 25.0%). However, the means of success for the same strategies were higher for the LS than for the LLS (word analysis: 1.28 vs. 0.52; word-form analogy: 0.90 vs. 0.23). This suggests that although lexically skilled readers employed local-word-based strategies less frequently than the less skilled readers, the LS readers benefited more from the same strategies when they did use them.

Differences were also observed between LS and LLS readers in terms of the frequency of evaluative strategies. LS readers used self-inquiry and verifying more frequently than LLS readers did (self-inquiry: 70.6% vs. 29.4%; verifying: 73% vs. 27%). The means of success for these two strategies were 1.16 and 1.55 for the LS readers and 1.10 and 1.40 for the LLS readers. A comparison of the mean of success across the two groups showed no significant difference for these strategies, suggesting that the two strategies produced similar rates of success in both LS and LLS groups. It is important to note that verifying obtained the highest mean of success in both groups. This result suggests the important contribution of this strategy to successful inferencing.

Successful use of monitoring strategies was also significantly different for the two groups. Although LS and LLS readers used this strategy with equal frequency, the mean of success for monitoring is significantly higher for the former than for the latter group. This finding suggests that the LS readers benefited more from monitoring strategies than the LLS readers did.

As a final step in the analysis, a two-way chi-square test was conducted on the proportions of strategy types and the two groups (lexically skilled and lexically less skilled readers). This analysis produced a significant chi-square value (x2 = 33.157, df = 6, p < 0.000), suggesting that the degree to which learners used different strategies was significantly related to their degree of lexical proficiency.

The above findings suggest an important link between learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge and their use of lexical inferencing [End Page 120] strategies. In particular, they show that lexically skilled and less skilled readers differed not only in the type of strategies used, but also in how effectively they use them. The reason for this advantage for the LS readers may be partly related to their greater depth as well as breadth of word knowledge: their richer lexical knowledge may make them better able to make use of the potential cues available in the text and co-text. The following two excerpts illustrate the inferencing attempts made by lexically skilled and lexically less skilled students:

LLS: '... and the smell of sewage in their nose ... in their nose ... their toes ...' 'the toes and the smell of sewage in their nose,' 'their toes ...' umm ... because it is in their nose, I think ... 'between their toes ...' I am not sure ... because something in their nose their ... mmm ... is ... mmm ... maybe it's their ... it's their ... I'm not sure. 'the smell of sewage ...' it's the smell ... the sewage it is something ... there is some smell ... may be sew ... I'm not sure ...

LS: 'Sewage in their nose ...' 'smell of their sewage in their nose ...' 'have dust between their toes and the smell of sewage in their nose ...' I think, there are a lot of dirty things around the city and that those things are making smells, and the smell goes to their noses, so sewage are things like dirty things, garbage, like, according to this ... 'and the smell of the sewage in their nose.'

Yes.

In the first example, the student's efforts seem to be mostly concentrated on the random repetition of certain words or parts of the text. The learner does not seem to be focusing on the meaning in the text, since the learner's repetitions pick up on inappropriate meaning boundaries in the text (e.g., '... in their nose ... their toes ... the toes and the smell of sewage in their nose,' ' their toes ...').In the second example, the student seems to be more focused. There are fewer random repetitions, and, when repeating, the student repeats a bigger meaningful section of the text that includes the word and the context in which the word has occurred. The student seems to have made better use of the information available in the text than the previous student. This is evident from the semantic connections the student has established between the different sections of the text and from the conclusion that the word 'sewage' should be related to dirty things because there are dirty things around the city. Finally, after inferring the meaning of the word, the student evaluates it by rereading the same portion of the text and trying to verify the accuracy of that inferencing attempt. [End Page 121]

Contributions of depth of vocabulary knowledge and degree of lexical inferencing strategy use to inferential success

The analyses described so far focused on the difference between LS and LLS readers in their use of different types of lexical inferencing strategies. The results show that lexically skilled and less skilled readers indeed differed significantly, both in terms of the type of strategies they used and in terms of how effectively they used them. The next series of analyses examined the collective and the independent contributions to inferential success of depth of vocabulary knowledge and degree of lexical inferencing attempts. To this end, a series of multiple regression analyses was conducted in which inferential success was used as a dependent (criterion) variable and the learner's degree of lexical inferencing strategy use and depth of vocabulary knowledge were used as independent (predictor) variables. Inferential success represented each learner's total scores of success for the 10 target words; depth of vocabulary knowledge consisted of the learners' scores on the depth of vocabulary knowledge test; and degree of strategy use consisted of composite scores of the various types of strategies used. Composite scores were calculated from the sum of standardized (z) scores of the frequency of each strategy type used. Z-scores were used because the raw frequency scores indicate how the strategy use of one student compares with that of another student who has used different types and degrees of strategies.

Separate forced-entry, hierarchical, multiple regressions were performed in which each of the predictor variables was entered into the regression equation in different orders. To determine the contribution of any of the independent variables over and above the contribution of the other, that variable was entered into the regression model after the other one. Any variance remaining for the second variable in the equation was then considered the unique variance of that variable, not being accounted for by the variables already in the equation (Kerlinger & Pedhazur, 1973).

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analyses are presented in Table 4. The first section of the table (marked A) displays the results of the regressions where degree of lexical inferencing strategy use was entered first into the equation, followed by depth of vocabulary [End Page 122] knowledge. These analyses examined whether depth of vocabulary knowledge could explain any variance in inferential success after the variance attributable to lexical inferencing attempts was partialled out. The second section (marked B) displays the results where degree of lexical inferencing strategy use was entered after depth of vocabulary knowledge. These analyses examined whether degree of lexical inferencing attempts could explain any significant proportion of variance after the variance attributable to depth of vocabulary knowledge was partialled out. The column labelled 'R2 change' in the table shows the magnitude of the contribution of each variable at the point where that variable was entered into the analysis.

Multiple regression analyses using degree of lexical inferencing strategy use and depth of vocabulary knowledge as predictor variables and lexical inferencing success as criterion variable
Click for larger view
Table 4
Multiple regression analyses using degree of lexical inferencing strategy use and depth of vocabulary knowledge as predictor variables and lexical inferencing success as criterion variable

As Table 4 shows, when entered on the first step, lexical inferencing strategy use accounted for 20% of the variance (p < 0.05) in lexical inferencing success. Entered on the second step, overall depth of vocabulary knowledge accounted for an additional and significant proportion of variance in lexical inferencing success (17%, p < 0.05). In the next series of analyses, depth of vocabulary knowledge, entered on the first step, explained a significant proportion of variance in inferential success (33%, p < 0.01). Entered on the second step, degree of lexical inferencing strategy accounted for only 4% of variance, which was not significant.

The results of these analyses suggest that of the two predictor variables, learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge was a much stronger predictor of lexical inferencing success than degree of strategy use. Thus, although there is a relationship between the use of lexical inferencing strategy and success, the effectiveness of lexical inferencing attempts is significantly mediated by the learner's depth of vocabulary knowledge.

Discussion and conclusions

This study examined the relationship between ESL learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge and their ability to derive word meaning from context. In particular, it examined how learners' depth of vocabulary knowledge relates to the degree and type of lexical inferencing strategies they use and how this relationship affects learners' success in inferring word meaning from context. Results indicate a significant link between [End Page 123] depth of vocabulary knowledge and the type and degree of lexical inferencing strategy use. They show that (a) those who had stronger depth of vocabulary knowledge used certain types of lexical inferencing strategies more frequently than those who had weaker depth of vocabulary knowledge; (b) those who had stronger depth of vocabulary knowledge used certain types of lexical inferencing strategies more effectively than those who had weaker depth of vocabulary knowledge; and (c) depth of vocabulary knowledge made a significant contribution to inferential success, over and above the contribution made by the learner's degree of inferencing strategy use. These findings add to and confirm the literature in both L1 and L2 learning concerning the central role of vocabulary knowledge in lexical inferencing, and support the hypothesis that lexical inferencing is a meaning construction process that depends heavily on the richness of the learner's semantic and conceptual system (Fukkink, Blok, & de Glopper, 2001; de Bot et al., 1997). They are also consistent with the theoretical view that the ability to make use of contextual clues in inferencing depends, to a large extent, on having an adequate knowledge base in place, in this case a threshold of vocabulary knowledge (Coady, Magoto, Hubbard, Graney, & Mokhtari, 1993; Laufer, 1988, 1992, 1996; Nation, 1993).

It is usually assumed that a major factor affecting lexical inferencing is the ability to make effective use of contextual clues (Dubin & Olshtain, 1993; Haastrup, 1991; Huckin & Bloch, 1993; Nagy, 1997; Nagy, Anderson, & Herman, 1987; Nagy & Scott, 2000; Parry, 1993). However, there is conflicting evidence in the literature as to whether students who have stronger linguistic skills can benefit more from the use of context than those who have weaker linguistic skills (Frantzen, 2003). Some studies have found that no matter what level of language proficiency learners have, context may not help them much in successfully inferring the meaning of unknown words (Bensoussan & Laufer, 1984). Other studies suggest that learners' level of language proficiency plays a major role in using context effectively (Haastrup, 1991; Laufer & Sim, 1985; Morrison, 1996). Morrison (1996), for example, found that those students who were linguistically more proficient made many more correct lexical guesses (74%) than less proficient learners, who made only 34% correct guesses. The present study's findings are consistent with those of Haastrup (1991), Laufer and Sim (1985), and Morrison (1996): in this study, of a total of 51 successful inferences, 35 (68.6%) were made by the lexically skilled readers and only 16 (31.4%) were made by lexically less skilled readers.

The analysis also showed a difference in the types of strategies used by the LS and LLS students. The students who were more proficient in [End Page 124] terms of depth of vocabulary knowledge made more frequent and effective use of evaluative and context-based strategies, including verifying, self-inquiry, and section repeating, than those who were lexically less skilled. On the other hand, LLS students made more use of local-word-based strategies such as analysis, word repeating, and word-form analogy, though the LS students benefited more from these same strategies when they did use them. These findings may explain the discrepancy, in previous research, in results pertaining to the relative importance of local-word-based strategies. For example, Huckin and Bloch (1993) report that one of the major and successful strategy types their learners used was analytic word-clue strategies. Other studies (Frantzen, 2003; Haynes, 1993; Morrison, 1996; Parry, 1993, 1997) did not report such an advantage for local-word-based strategies. One factor that may explain this difference could be the learner's depth of vocabulary knowledge. It is possible that learners who participated in Huckin and Bloch's study, because they were graduate students studying at an English-speaking university, had a stronger depth of vocabulary knowledge and, hence, had a better knowledge of the different components that make up the words. This enhanced knowledge of word components could have increased their ability to use and benefit from word-based strategies.

Another finding of this research has to do with the relative contributions of depth of vocabulary knowledge and degree of lexical inferencing strategy use. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that while the degree of lexical inferencing attempts explained some proportion of the variance in lexical inferencing success when entered first in the equation, its contribution became insignificant when the contribution related to the depth of vocabulary knowledge was partialled out. This finding, as well as those reported above relating to the difference between lexically skilled and less skilled readers in type of strategy use, suggests that while lexical inferencing strategy plays a role in lexical inferencing success, the degree and effectiveness of these strategic attempts are heavily mediated by the learner's depth of lexical and semantic knowledge.

Overall, the above findings seem to support approaches to lexical inferencing that emphasize the richness of the learner's semantic and conceptual systems. Thus, they are consistent with the view that inferring the meanings of unfamiliar words from context depends on how successfully learners construct appropriate semantic features for an unknown word (de Bot et al., 1997). In their study, de Bot et al. (1997) found evidence for the use of multiple sources of information during lexical inferencing. They found that learners frequently appealed to their [End Page 125] knowledge of syntax, morphology, word derivation, and association. These knowledge sources are important components of depth of lexical knowledge (Nation, 1990; Richards, 1976). Thus, it can be assumed that those learners who possess a deeper lexical knowledge have better access to these knowledge sources and, hence, can construct a more accurate semantic representation of the unknown word during lexical inferencing than those who do not.

Implications and directions for further research

The present study supports the findings of previous research that L2 learners need good vocabulary knowledge to be able to successfully derive word meanings from context (Coady et al., 1993; Haynes & Baker, 1993; Laufer, 1997; Nation, 1990, 2001). Strong evidence exists in the field of L1 vocabulary research regarding the effectiveness of learning vocabulary from context. Research in L2 contexts, however, does not provide such strong support for lexical inferencing (Bensoussan & Laufer, 1984; Haastrup, 1991). Based on the findings of this study, it could be argued that the difference in effectiveness of lexical inferencing between L2 and L1 (native) readers may also be related to the difference between the two in terms of their depth of vocabulary knowledge. L1 readers are different from L2 readers not only in terms of their breadth of vocabulary knowledge (Nation, 2001) but also in terms of the totality of lexical representation for each lexical entry in their lexicon. The different types of syntactic, semantic, and morphological information forming a lexical representation of a lexical entry in a learner's L1 are more strongly and highly integrated than those in the L2 (Jiang, 2000). This provides L1 learners with a rich lexical knowledge network that can be drawn upon, during lexical inferencing, to integrate information across and within sentences and to generate accurate syntactic and semantic inferences about words. Thus, in order to benefit from lexical inferencing, L2 learners must also have such a robust lexical knowledge base in place (Coady et al., 1993; Hirsh & Nation, 1992; Laufer, 1988, 1992, 1996; Nation, 1993). One way of achieving this goal would be to establish a thorough vocabulary learning program that integrates extensive exposure to language and learning vocabulary from context with direct and systematic vocabulary instruction, particularly in the early stages of L2 acquisition (see Paribakht & Wesche, 1997; Zimmerman, 1997).

Another related implication is for strategy training. In this study lexically less skilled readers rarely used context-based evaluative strategies, suggesting that this strategy should be emphasized in teaching [End Page 126] lexical inferencing strategies. However, one of the key issues in the literature on strategy training is whether learners can be trained to use lexical inferencing strategies effectively. While some studies have found a positive effect for strategy training on reading comprehension and inferencing ability (e.g., Fraser, 1999; Kern, 1989), others have failed to produce such strong effects (e.g., Barnett, 1988). The present study found that the type and effectiveness of lexical inferencing strategies learners use are significantly related to the learner's depth of vocabulary knowledge. Therefore, the role of this factor must be considered in lexical inferencing strategy use and training. Clearly, further research is needed to investigate empirically the role of this factor in strategy training. Previous studies have suggested that an important factor affecting success in lexical inferencing strategy use is the learner's language proficiency (Kern, 1989). However, language proficiency is multifaceted, and it is possible that some dimensions of that knowledge may play a greater role than others in helping the learner to infer the meaning of an unfamiliar word from context. Thus, further research is needed to investigate the relationship between the different components of language proficiency and lexical inferencing; for example, the role of size of vocabulary knowledge versus depth of vocabulary knowledge in deriving word meaning from context. Size and depth of vocabulary knowledge have been shown to differentially affect L2 reading comprehension (Qian, 1999). Are these variables also different in their contributions to lexical inferencing strategy use and success? Further research is also needed to address the role of other dimensions of linguistic knowledge, such as grammatical knowledge, morphological knowledge, and discourse knowledge, in L2 lexical inferencing.

Another point is in order. Research has suggested that the kinds of lexical inferencing strategies L2 learners use depend heavily on the nature of the word as well as the text containing the word (Paribakht & Wesche, 1999). Therefore, before drawing any conclusions about the relative importance of the factors affecting lexical inferencing, further research must examine these processes in different contexts and with different types of texts and words.

Hossein Nassaji is Assistant Professor of Applied Linguistics in the Linguistics Department at the University of Victoria, British Columbia, Canada. His teaching and research interests include second language acquisition, focus on form in L2 classrooms, and L2 reading and vocabulary learning. His work has been published in a variety of journals, including Applied Linguistics, Applied Psycholinguistics, Language Learning, Modern Language Journal, TESOL Quarterly, Canadian Modern Language Review, and Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. [End Page 127]

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank John Read, who provided me with the Word-Associate Test used in the study. I would also like to thank the anonymous reviewers and the editor of the themed-based issue of the Canadian Modern Language Review, Patricia Raymond, for their careful reading and very helpful comments.

Endnotes

1. The test was supplied to the author by its developer, John Read, of Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand.

2. This is a reclassification of the strategies reported in Nassaji (2003b).

References

Anderson, J.C. (2000). Assessing reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P.D. Pearson (Ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255-292). New York: Longman.

Barnett, M.A. (1988). Reading through context: How real and perceived strategy use affects L2 comprehension. Modern Language Journal, 72, 50-62.

Bensoussan, M., & Laufer, B. (1984). Lexical guessing in context in EFL reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 7, 15-23.

Brown, C.M. (1993). Factors affecting the acquisition of vocabulary: Frequency and saliency of words. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 263-286). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Brown, G., & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Buikema, J.L., & Graves, M.F. (1993). Teaching students to use context clues to infer word meanings. Journal of Reading, 36, 450-457.

Chapelle, C. (1994). Are C-tests valid measures for L2 vocabulary research? Second Language Research, 10, 157-187.

Chern, C. (1993). Chinese students' word-solving strategies in reading in English. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 67-85). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Citizenship and Immigration Canada. (1996). Canadian Language Benchmarks: English as a second language for adults / English as a second language for literacy learners [working document]. Ottawa, ON: Supply and Service Canada. [End Page 128]

Coady, J., Magoto, J., Hubbard, P., Graney, J., & Mokhtari, K. (1993). High frequency vocabulary and reading proficiency in ESL readers. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 217-226). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

de Bot, K., Paribakht, T.S., & Wesche, M.B. (1997). Toward a lexical pro-

cessing model for the study of second language vocabulary acquisition: Evidence from ESL reading. Studies of Second Language Acquisition, 19, 309-329.

Dubin, F., & Olshtain, E. (1993). Predicting word meanings from contextual clues: Evidence from L1 readers. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 181-202). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Frantzen, D. (2003). Factors affecting how second language Spanish students derive meaning from context. Modern Language Journal, 87, 168-199.

Fraser, C.A. (1999). Lexical processing strategy use and vocabulary learning through reading. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 225-241.

Fukkink, R.G., Blok, H., & de Glopper, K. (2001). Deriving word meaning from written context: A multicomponential skill. Language Learning, 51, 477-496.

Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1990). Referential processing in reading: Focusing on roles and individuals. In D.A. Balota, G.B. Flores d'Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 465-484). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Graesser, A.C., & Bower, G.H. (Eds.). (1990). Inferences and text comprehension. New York: Academic Press.

Graesser, A.C., & Zwaan, R.A. (1995). Inference generation and the construction of situation models. In C.A. Weaver, S. Mannes, & C.R. Fletcher III (Eds.), Discourse comprehension: Essays in Honor of Walter Kintsch (pp. 117-139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Greidanus, T., & Nienhuis, L. (2001). Testing the quality of word knowledge in a second language by means of word associations: Types of distractors and types of associations. Modern Language Journal, 85, 567-577.

Haastrup, K. (1991). Lexical inferencing procedures or talking about words: Receptive procedures in foreign language learning with special reference to English. Tubingen, Germany: Gunter Narr.

Haastrup, K., & Henriksen, B. (2000). Vocabulary acquisition: Acquiring depth of knowledge through network building. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 10, 221-240.

Haynes, M. (1993). Patterns and perils of guessing in second language reading. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 46-64). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Haynes, M., & Baker, I. (1993). American and Chinese readers learning from lexical familiarizations in English text. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 130-150). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. [End Page 129]

Henriksen, B. (1999). Three dimensions of vocabulary development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 303-317.

Hirsh, D., & Nation, P. (1992). What vocabulary size is needed to read unsimplified texts for pleasure? Reading in a Foreign Language, 8, 689-696.

Huckin, T., & Bloch, J. (1993). Strategies for inferring word meaning from context: A cognitive model. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes, & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 153-178). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Huckin, T., & Coady, J. (1999). Incidental vocabulary acquisition in a second language: A review. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 181-193.

Huckin, T., & Jin, Z. (1987). Inferring word-meaning from context: A study in second language acquisition. In F. Marshall, A. Miller, & Z. Zhang (Eds.), Proceedings of the Third Eastern States Conference on Linguistics. Columbus: The Ohio State University.

Jenkins, J., Matlock, B., & Slocum, T. (1989). Two approaches to vocabulary instruction: The teaching of individual word meanings and practice in deriving word meaning from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 215-235.

Jiang, N. (2000). Lexical representation and development in a second language. Applied Linguistics, 21, 47-77.

Joe, A. (1995). Text-based tasks and incidental vocabulary learning. Wellington, New Zealand: English Language Institute.

Kerlinger, F., & Pedhazur, E. (1973). Multiple regression in behavioral research. New York: Holt: Rinehart & Winston.

Kern, R.G. (1989). Second language reading instruction: Its effects on comprehension and word inference ability. Modern Language Journal, 73, 135-149.

Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 92, 163-182.

Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Laufer, B. (1988). What percentage of text-lexis is essential for comprehension? In C. Lauren & M. Nordmann (Eds.), Special language: From humans thinking to thinking machines (pp. 316-323). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

Laufer, B. (1992). How much lexis is necessary for reading comprehension? In H. Béjoint & P. Arnaud (Eds.), Vocabulary and applied linguistics (pp. 126-132). London: MacMillan. [End Page 130]

Laufer, B. (1996). The lexical threshold of second language reading comprehension: What it is and how it relates to L1 reading ability. In K. Sajavaara & C. Fairweather (Eds.), Approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 55-62). Jyvaskyla, Finland: University of Jyvaskyla.

Laufer, B. (1997). What's in a word that makes it hard or easy: Some intralexical factors that affect the learning of words. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 140-180). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Laufer, B., & Sim, D.D. (1985). Taking the easy way out: Non use and misuse of contextual clues in EFL reading comprehension. English Teaching Forum, 23, 7-10.

Liu, N., & Nation, I.S.P. (1985). Factors affecting guessing vocabulary in context. RELC Journal, 16, 33-42.

Meara, P. (1996). The dimensions of lexical competence. In G. Brown, K. Malmkjaer, & J. Williams (Eds.), Performance and competence in second language acquisition (pp. 35-53). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Monzo, A.E., & Calvo, M.G. (2002). Context constraints, prior vocabulary knowledge and on-line inferences in reading. Psicothema, 14, 357-362.

Morrison, L. (1996). Talking about words: A study of French as a second language learners' lexical inferencing procedures. Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 41-75.

Nagy, W. (1997). On the role of context in first- and second-language vocabulary learning. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 64-83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nagy, W., Anderson, R.C., & Herman, P.A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 237-270.

Nagy, W., & Scott, J. (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 269-284). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Nassaji, H. (2002). Schema theory and knowledge-based processes in second language reading comprehension: A need for alternative perspectives. Language Learning, 52, 439-481.

Nassaji, H. (2003a). Higher-level and lower-level text processing skills in advanced ESL reading comprehension. Modern Language Journal, 87, 261-276.

Nassaji, H. (2003b). L2 vocabulary learning from context: Strategies, knowledge sources, and their relationship with success in 12 lexical inferencing. TESOL Quarterly, 37, 645-670.

Nation, I.S.P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. New York: Newbury House. [End Page 131]

Nation, I.S.P. (1993). Vocabulary size, growth, and use. In R. Schreuder & B. Weltens (Eds.), The bilingual lexicon (pp. 115-134). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Nation, I.S.P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Paribakht, T.S., & Wesche, M. (1993). Reading comprehension and second language development in a comprehension-based ESL program. TESL Canada Journal, 11, 9-29.

Paribakht, T.S., & Wesche, M. (1997). Vocabulary enhancement activities and reading for meaning in second language vocabulary acquisition. In J. Coady & T. Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition: A rationale for pedagogy (pp. 174-199). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Paribakht, T.S., & Wesche, M. (1999). Reading and 'incidental' L2 vocabulary acquisition: An introspective study of lexical inferencing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 195-224.

Parry, K. (1993). Too many words: Learning the vocabulary of an academic subject. In T. Huckin, M. Haynes & J. Coady (Eds.), Second language reading and vocabulary learning (pp. 109-129). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Parry, K. (1997). Vocabulary and comprehension: Two portraits. In J. Coady & T. Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition: A rationale for pedagogy (pp. 55-68). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Pressley, M., & Afferbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Pressley, M., Borkowski, J.G., & Schneider, W. (1987). Cognitive strategies: Good strategy users coordinate metacognition and knowledge. Annals of Child Development, 4, 89-129.

Pulido, D. (2003). Modeling the role of second language proficiency and topic familiarity in second language incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading. Language Learning, 53, 233-284.

Qian, D. (1998). Depth of vocabulary knowledge: Assessing its role in adults' reading comprehension in English as a second language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, OISE/University of Toronto.

Qian, D. (1999). Assessing the roles of depth and breadth of vocabulary knowledge in reading comprehension. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 56, 282-308.

Qian, D. (2002). Investigating the relationship between vocabulary knowledge and academic reading performance: An assessment perspective. Language Learning, 52, 513-536.

Read, J. (1993). The development of a new measure of L2 vocabulary knowledge. Language Testing, 10, 355-371.

Read, J. (1997). Vocabulary and testing. In M. McCarthy (Ed.), Vocabulary: [End Page 132]

Description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 303-320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Read, J. (2000). Assessing vocabulary. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Richards, J.C. (1976). The role of vocabulary learning. TESOL Quarterly, 10, 77-89.

Sternberg, R.J., & Powell, J.S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878-893.

Verspoor, M., & Lowie, W. (2003). Making sense of polysemous words. Language Learning, 53, 547-586.

Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T.S. (1996). Assessing second language vocabulary knowledge: Depth versus breadth. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 13-40.

Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T.S. (1999). Introduction: Incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition: Theory, current research and instructional implications. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 175-180.

Whitney, P. (1987). Psychological theories of elaborative inferences: Implications for schema-theoretic views of comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 299-310.

Zimmerman, C. (1997). Do reading and interactive vocabulary instruction make a difference? An empirical study. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 121-140.

Appendix A

Health in the Rich World and in the Poor

An American journalist, Dorothy Thompson, criticises the rich world's health programmes in the poor world. She describes her trip to Africa where she got food poisoning and her friend malaria:

"The town is very dirty. All the people are hot, have dust between their toes and the smell of sewage in their noses. We both fell ill, and at ten o'clock in the morning I got frightened and took my friend to the only private hospital in town, where you have to pay. After being treated by a doctor, we caught the next aeroplane home.

"Now, I believe that the money of the World Health Organisation (WHO) should be spent on bringing health to all people of the world and not on expensive doctors and hospitals for the few who can pay. But when we ourselves become ill, our beliefs waver. After we came back to the States we thought a lot about our reaction to this sudden meeting with heath care in a poor country. When assessing modern medicine, we often forget that without more money for food and clean water to drink, it is impossible to fight the diseases that are caused by infections. [End Page 133]

"Doctors seem to overlook this fact. They ought to spend much time thinking about why they themselves do not contract some of the serious and infectious diseases that so many of their patients die from. They do not realize that an illness must find a body that is weak either because of stress or hunger. People are killed by the conditions they live under, the lack of food and money and the squalor. Doctors should analyze why people become ill rather than take such a keen interest in the curative effect of medicine.

"In the rich world many diseases are caused by affluence. The causes of heart diseases, for instance, are far from being mysterious and unfathomable - they are as well known as the causes of tuberculosis. Other diseases are due to hazards in the natural conditions in which we live. Imagine the typical American worker on his death-bed: every cell permeated with such things as chemicals and radio-active materials. Such symptoms are true signs of an unhealthy world."

Source: Haastrup (1991, p. 234). © 1991 Gunter Narr Verlag Tübingen. Used by permission.



Share